Neuroimaging of emotional dysregulation in multiple sclerosis: relationship with alexithymia

Original Article
Issue
2021/04
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.4414/sanp.2021.03216
Swiss Arch Neurol Psychiatr Psychother. 2021;172:w03216

Affiliations
a Neurology Unit, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals (HUG), Geneva, Switzerland
b Laboratory for Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, Department of Fundamental Neurosciences, University of Geneva, Campus Biotech, Geneva, Switzerland
c Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
d Institution de Lavigny, Lavigny, Switzerland
e Laboratory for the study of Emotion Elicitation and Expression (E3 Lab), Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
f Swiss Centre for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Campus Biotech, Geneva, Switzerland
g Centre for Brain Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, ColchesterUnited Kingdom
h University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland

Published on 29.07.2021

Compensatory mechanisms in minimally disabled MS patients, which may serve to preserve homeostasis of amygdala activity and affective state.

Introduction

Impairments in cognition and social functioning are increasingly recognised in multiple sclerosis (MS) and contribute to handicaps in everyday life [1]. Such deficits may result from long-range disconnections induced by demyelinating lesions due to the disease [2, 3]. In particular, associated emotion perception disorders have been linked to dysfunctional activity within a network of brain regions comprising the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the amygdala [47], respectively involved in the cognitive and affective evaluation of behaviourally relevant events [810].
Emotion regulation abilities are a key component of affective processes and psychological health. These may be impaired in MS and contribute to behavioural difficulties in patients and include alexithymia [1114], characterised by the inability to identify and describe ones’ own feelings. Emotion regulation mechanisms, such as cognitive reappraisal, primarily involve top-down modulatory inputs from the lateral PFC to the amygdala [15], leading to a decreased response of the amygdala to negative stimuli [1620]. Healthy alexithymic subjects typically use poorly efficient strategies to regulate their emotions [21, 22] and display a reduced amygdala response to negative stimuli [23]. On the other hand, other regions within a distributed prefrontal-striatal-pallidalthalamic-limbic network also take part in the regulation of amygdala activity [15, 20, 24, 25]. For instance, in healthy participants, as well as in other neurodegenerative disorders, lower striatum activity has been repeatedly implicated in alexithymia [13, 23, 26]. However, in MS, current knowledge underlying the neural substrates of alexithymia is restricted to the sole finding of increased atrophy in the posterior corpus callosum [27], tentatively linked to impaired communication of affective information between the two hemispheres. On the other hand, disconnection lesions commonly associated with MS might potentially disrupt top- down modulation of emotion processing by cognitive reappraisal, a hypothesis that remains to be explored.
Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine brain circuits mobilised in MS patients during emotion perception and regulation [28, 29]. Patients were probed for alexithymia as well as other affective disorders, enabling us to investigate their relationship with emotion perception and regulation abilities, and concomitant brain activity. We hypothesised that alexithymia in MS might be associated with dysfunctional patterns of activity in prefrontal areas and amygdala during reappraisal due to impaired connectivity between these areas as a result of white-matter lesions.

Methods

Standard protocol approvals, registrations and patient consent

This study was approved by the local ethics committee (University of Lausanne, Switzerland, CCER 27.07.10, “Etude en IRMf des capacités de régulation des patients avec sclérose en plaque”). All participants signed a written informed consent form, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants

Nineteen patients with relapsing-remitting MS and minimal disability (McDonald's diagnostic criteria [30]) took part in this study (see table 1). Inclusion criteria were: (1) mild to moderate neurological disability but unimpaired ambulation (Expanded Disability Status Scale [EDSS] ≤2.5 in all cases [31];); (2) no clinical relapse and no corticosteroid therapy for at least 6 weeks before inclusion in the study; (3) no other neurological diagnosis, major depression or psychiatric illness according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM5) criteria; (4) no use of antidepressant, anxiolytic or antipsychotic drugs; and (5) no evidence of cognitive impairment on a complete neuropsychological examination.
Table 1
Demographic and clinical data.
 Multiple sclerosis patientsControlsp-value
Number of males/females8/118/12 
Age (years)33.5 ± 433.5 ± 4.50.60
Education (years)14.4 ± 2.214.9 ± 2.50.59
Handedness (left/right-handed)2/170/20 
Disease duration (years)8.4 ± 5.2 
EDSS1.5 (1–3) 
Disability progression0.27 ± 0.1 
Anxiety/depression   
Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale-A7.2 ± 3.17.1 ± 3.40.88
Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale-D3.3 ± 2.92.8 ± 2.30.51
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory41.9 ± 10.646 ± 10.30.23
Affective questionnaires   
Behavioral Inhibition System scale30.8 ± 4.230 ± 4.20.54
Behavioral Approach System scale64.4 ± 7.262.7 ± 80.49
Positive and Negative Affect scale   
(1) Positive59.3 ± 13.357.6 ± 10.30.65
(2) Negative37.5 ± 12.539.9 ± 9.40.49
Toronto Alexithymia Scale   
(1) Difficulty describing feelings17.9 ± 6.213.5 ± 40.01
(2) Difficulty identifying feeling14.2 ± 3.411.1 ± 3.70.009
(3) Externally oriented thinking21.2 ± 4.615.6 ± 30.000*
(4) Total score53.4 ± 9.140.1 ± 80.000*
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire   
(1) Reappraisal28.7 ± 4.929.5 ± 5.90.64
(2) Suppression15.1 ± 6.312.5 ± 4.90.16
Cognitive measures (BRB-N)   
Selective reminding 66 (54-70) 
(1) Long Term Storage62 (34–71) 0.15
(2) Consistent long term storage55 (21–71)59 (42–70)0.14
(3) Delayed recall11 (5–12)12 (9–12)0.72
10/36 spatial recall 21 (13–28) 
(1) Total24 (16–30) 0.21
(2) Delayed9 (3–10)8 (5–10)0.70
Paced Auditory Serial Addition test57 (31–60)56 (23–60)0.96
Symbol Digit Modalities test57 (38–73)59 (46–77)0.31
Word list generation27 (15–35)32 (21–37)0.02
Mean ± SD (t-tests), except for EDSS and cognitive measures (median (range); Mann-Whitney U-tests). * significant after Bonferroni correction.
Fourteen patients were treated with disease-modifying therapies (interferon β-1a or 1b in 8 cases, natalizumab in 3, fingolimod in 2, glatiramer acetate in 1) for a mean period of 3.8 years (ranging from 5 months to 10 years).
Twenty healthy subjects with no previous history of neurological or psychiatric disorder were recruited as a control group (HCs; see table 1). Gender was balanced within and between groups, but not included as a covariate in our analyses given the small sample size precluding reliable conclusions about gender differences.

Affective measures

In addition to the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS [32];), a set of questionnaires was administered in order to evaluate affective state more globally, including the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), the Behavioral Inhibition and Approach System Scale (BISBAS), the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS), and anxiety and depression scales (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; State-Trait Anxiety Inventory).

Cognitive measures

The Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N [33, 34]) was administered to all patients and 15 out of 20 controls, to assess verbal memory (Selective Reminding Test [SRT]), spatial memory (10/36 Spatial Recall Test), sustained attention/information processing speed (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test [PASAT], Symbol Digit Modalities Test [SDMT]), and verbal fluency on semantic cues (Word List Generation [WLG]).

Experimental protocol

Participants underwent event-related fMRI during emotion perception and regulation, following a well-established protocol described extensively elsewhere [28, 29]. The task was controlled using E-prime 1.0 (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh). Subjects viewed photographs of emotional scenes (positive or negative valence from the International Affective Pictures System) under two conditions: “spontaneous viewing” and “reappraisal”. The scenes involved social or non-social contents in equal distribution. In the “viewing” condition, for each picture, subjects rated the valence and intensity of their spontaneous emotional feeling, on a scale ranging from −2 (very negative) to +2 (very positive) in steps of 1, with 0 being neutral. In the “reappraisal” condition, they had to regulate their emotions by reinterpreting the meaning of the scene in such a way as to reduce its affective significance, for example by imagining the pictures were made up or taken from a movie scene, and hence unreal or not truly emotional. Again, participants rated the valence and intensity of their emotions, this time after emotional reappraisal. This type of emotion regulation strategy typically leads to a diminution of emotional intensities (scores closer to 0 [28, 29]).

MRI acquisition

Data were acquired on a 3T MRI system (Trio TIM, Siemens, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. Whole-brain functional images were collected with a susceptibility weighted echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (repetition time/echo time/flip angle = 2200 ms/30 ms/90°; field of view = 216 × 216 mm). Thirty-five axial slices were acquired in an interleaved ascending manner (slice thickness = 3 mm, interslice gap = 0.3 mm; voxel size = 3 mm3 isotropic). High-resolution anatomical images were acquired using a T1-weighted, 3D sequence (MPRAGE; repetition time/echo time/flip angle = 2300 ms/2.98 ms/9°; field of view = 240 × 160 mm; slice thickness = 1mm; interslice gap=0.5 mm; voxel size = 1 mm3 isotropic). Visual stimuli were back projected on a screen and head movements were prevented using an ergonomic air head-cushion.

MRI data preprocessing

Functional MRI data were preprocessed and analysed with SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) in a conventional manner. All functional volumes underwent slice timing correction, spatial realignment, spatial normalisation to an EPI brain template, resampling to 3-mm isotropic voxels and spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel, 8 mm FWHM). The data were scaled globally and a high-pass filter (cut-off = 128 s) was applied prior to data modelling. The anatomical volumes were spatially co-registered to the mean functional image resulting from spatial realignment.

First-level general linear model

Functional data were modelled using a generalised linear model (GLM) with one regressor for each combination of task condition (viewing, reappraisal), emotional valence (positive, negative) and social content (social, non-social; not analysed in the current paper), yielding eight regressors convolved with a canonical haemodynamic function. Six additional regressors were included to account for head movement during scanning, using realignment parameters from the pre-processing step. Main contrasts of interest were defined by these eight conditions minus the implicit baseline activity.

Second-level group analysis

Contrast maps were incorporated in a flexible factorial design of analysis of variance (ANOVA) for group analyses. The critical comparisons were performed by means of t-tests between each group of participants (HC, MS) for each emotion condition (viewing, reappraisal), and each combination of emotion condition and valence (pooled across social content). Brain areas commonly engaged by HC and MS participants were explored using conjunction analyses (p <0.05 family-wise error [few] corrected). For between-group contrasts, whole-brain analyses were performed with a threshold p <0.005 uncorrected at voxel level and a minimum cluster extent k = 50 voxels, providing a reasonable balance between type I and type II errors since the precise onset of affective mechanisms is unknown at each trial and may vary in subjects from a clinical population [28]. We further provide significance at threshold p <0.001 for the sake of completeness.
Significant peak activity in the second-level t-tests was observed in a number of brain areas involved in emotion perception and regulation, including lateral PFC. Modulation of functional connectivity for this region of interest was further examined using the generalised psychophysiological interaction (PPI) approach (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/gppi [35];). We further restricted the PPI analyses on relevant regions involved in emotion regulation, based on independently defined coordinates obtained by automated meta-analysis (http://neurosynth.org; keyword: emotion regulation, map threshold p <0.01 false discovery rate [FDR] corrected, used as an inclusive mask), with a statistical threshold p <0.005 uncorrected at voxel level (minimum cluster size = 10 voxels) [28]. Changes in connectivity between lateral PFC (sphere, 8 mm diameter) and brain areas involved in emotion regulation was computed for each combination of emotional condition (viewing, reappraisal) and valence (positive, negative) against implicit baseline activity for each subject. The resulting contrast maps were then incorporated in a factorial ANOVA at the second-level for between-group analyses.

Results

Questionnaire data

MS patients scored comparably to HCs in all questionnaires with the notable exception of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS), where patients reported higher alexithymia scores for all subscales compared with controls (table 1). Among patients, 13 out of 19 (68%) had significant or probable alexithymia as assessed by the TAS (cut-off score>51). In patients, the total alexithymia score correlated positively with the disease duration (Spearman rho = 0.644, p = 0.003), HAD-D (rho = 0.688, p = 0.001) and STAI (rho = 0.63, p = 0.004), whereas it correlated negatively with the BAS (rho = −0.67, p = 0.002). These data corroborate previous reports highlighting the higher prevalence of alexithymic disorders in MS compared with the normal population, as well as their association with depression and anxiety [36].

Cognitive measures

Patients did not differ from controls in the cognitive tests, except for verbal fluency (Mann-Whitney U test, Z = 2.36, p = 0.02; table 1).

Emotional ratings during functional MRI

An analysis of variance (group, task, and valence as factors) revealed no significant differences in subjective emotional ratings of pictures between patients and controls (all p >0.13). The interaction between task and valence was significant (F(1,37) = 85.3; p <0.001), with a greater difference in ratings between positive and negative events during viewing (∆ = 2.4) than during reappraisal of emotional scenes (∆ = 1.7; t (1, 38) =9 .1; p <0.001). This indirectly confirms that all participants reliably regulated their emotional state according to the task instructions. No correlation was observed between emotional ratings and alexithymia scores (or other affective questionnaires) in the MS group (all p values >0.235).

Anatomical MRI data

In the MS group, reduced whole-brain white matter volume was associated with higher EDSS scores (Spearman rho = −0.474, p = 0.04) and higher alexithymia scores (TAS-1: rho = −0.619, p = 0.005; TAS-2: rho= −0.517, p = 0.023; TAS total: rho = − 0.599, p = 0.007). In controls, higher white matter volume was associated with more efficient regulation of negative emotions, i.e., correlating with more positive emotional ratings after reappraisal of negative scenes (rho = 0.56, p = 0.01). There was no such correlation in patients (rho = 0.048, p=.844).

Functional MRI data

Shared brain activity between MS patients and HC

During spontaneous viewing of emotional scenes, both patients and controls commonly engaged a widespread set of regions, including the ventrolateral PFC (orbital and opercular inferior frontal gyrus-IFG), insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), basal ganglia (dorsal striatum), and visual and sensorimotor areas, as well as posterior parietal cortex (conjunction analysis, p <0.05 FWE corrected; table 2A). During the reappraisal of emotional scenes, areas comparable to those during the viewing condition were again recruited in both groups (p <0.05 FWE corrected), except that the orbital IFG and the dorsal striatum were absent in this condition (table 2B).
Table 2
Conjunction analysis between MS and HC groups for emotion viewing.
 BAT-Value   MNI coordinatesCluster size
xyz
A. VIEWING      
Frontal/insular/subcortical      
L anterior insula 8.43−30224376
R anterior insula 6.6430244180
R orbital IFG475.933432-10 
L SMA / ACC611.8−4−6541646
R dorsal ACC327.4682040 
R opercular IFG456.64561222347
L putamen 6.34−2246119
R putamen 5.90226899
Motor/sensori-motor      
L precentral gyrus412.33−34−24542886
L postcentral gyrus2/310.98−46-3056 
L middle frontal gyrus69.10−54636346
R precentral gyrus / middle frontal gyrus65.9640−106226
R postcentral gyrus2/37.1542−2844342
Posterior parietal      
R SPL75.0814−665622
Visuo-perceptual      
R lingual gyrus3715.3928−48-89279
R fusiform gyrus1913.6528−62-8 
R middle occipital gyrus1813.3732−8614 
B. REAPPRAISAL      
Frontal/insular      
L dorsal ACC326.68−42242 
R opercular IFG446.92461228377
R triangular IFG454.9846341638
L anterior insula 5.57−2822631
Motor/sensori-motor      
L SMA611.30−6−6541448
R SMA610.512058 
L middle frontal gyrus69.34−52634352
L precentral gyrus412.29−36−22563251
L postcentral gyrus/IPL2/4010.91−40−3244 
R postcentral gyrus2/36.6546−2844197
Posterior parietal      
R SPL75.6814−6656141
R IPL75.2934−5450 
Visuo-perceptual      
lingual gyrus3715.7828−48-810210
R middle occipital gyrus1814.4232−8614 
R fusiform gyrus1914.0526−66-8 
ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; BA = Brodmann areas; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; L = left, R = right SLP = superior parietal lobule; SMA = supplementary motor area, .
p <0.05 familywise error corrected. Minimum cluster size = 20.

HC > MS patient comparisons

When viewing either negative or positive emotional scenes, the HC group showed higher activity in a distributed network involving the ventrolateral PFC (triangular and opercular IFG) and the inferior parietal cortex (IPL), together with sensorimotor and visual areas (p <0.005; fig. 1,table 3B). Similarly, during the reappraisal of emotional scenes, HC showed higher activity in dorsal fronto-parietal, sensorimotor, and visual areas, again irrespective of emotional content (p <0.005; fig. 1, table 3, C and D).
Figure 1
Healthy controls (HC) > MS patients, whole-brain contrast during the viewing and reappraisal of negative and positive emotional scenes (p <0.005 uncorrected, cluster size >50). L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere.
Table 3
Controls > MS patients, comparison of whole-brain activity during emotion viewing and reappraisal of negative and positive scenes.
 BAT-value   MNI coordinatesCluster size
x  yz
A. VIEWING NEGATIVE SCENES      
Frontal      
L triangular IFG*453.55−463228104
L opercular IFG*443.39−501424101
Parietal      
R supramarginal gyrus*404.1056-3036428
R angular gyrus*393.8842-6434129
L IPL*403.73−40-3636552
L supramarginal gyrus* 3.33−50-2624 
Sensori-motor      
L postcentral gyrus* 3.29−50-1050100
L precentral gyrus 2.99−40-256 
R paracentral lobule 3.0310-346059
L paracentral lobule 2.77−4-3258 
Visual      
L middle occipital gyrus* 3.85−20-5840126
R lingual gyrus*183.7322-76-6143
B. VIEWING POSITIVE SCENES      
Frontal      
L triangular IFG*453.82−443228148
R triangular IFG*453.5362202269
L opercular IFG*443.58−50142494
L superior frontal gyrus* 3.32−14364256
R ACC* 3.274442663
Parietal      
R supramarginal gyrus* 4.1948-3634739
R angular gyrus*394.0540-6234 
R IPL*403.6856-4444 
L supramarginal gyrus* 3.77−46-2826759
Sensori-motor      
R middle frontal gyrus*63.6542-45681
L middle frontal gyrus* 3.24−38164850
R SMA*43.374-2262252
R paracentral lobule* 3.3210-3460 
L paracentral lobule43.07−4-3258 
Visual      
L middle occipital gyrus* 3.98−20-5834146
R lingual gyrus*184.3222-76-6221
C. REAPRAISAL OF NEGATIVE SCENES      
Frontal      
L triangular IFG*453.76−423226182
L opercular IFG*443.41−46142292
R ACC*323.72144624121
Temporal-parietal      
R superior temporal gyrus*224.8766-2410512
R supramarginal gyrus* 3.6952-3432 
L temporo-parietal junction*413.34−42-3416313
L supramarginal gyrus* 3.24−56-2824 
R IPL*403.2956-524662
Sensori-motor      
L postcentral gyrus*64.22−50-848130
Visual      
R lingual gyrus*185.2620-78-6763
L calcarine gyrus183.07−14-62494
L lingual gyrus182.95−6-70-2 
D. REAPRAISAL OF POSITIVE SCENES      
Frontal      
L triangular IFG*483.56−443226146
R opercular IFG*454.00381616123
R ACC* 3.348462498
L middle frontal gyrus323.01−4054052
Temporal-parietal      
R superior temporal gyrus*224.3366-2410104
R supramarginal gyrus* 3.29 -363074
R angular gyrus*393.6142-623279
L inferior parietal lobule*33.51−42-303491
L supramarginal gyrus 2.84−50-2626 
Sensori-motor      
L postcentral gyrus*63.92−50-8 189
R precentral/middle frontal gyrus*63.7440-6 70
Visual      
R lingual gyrus*185.1720-76  
R fusiform gyrus*193.7436-70 901
L fusiform gyrus*373.79−18-46 129
BA = Brodmann areas, IFG = inferior frontal gyrus, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, SMA = supplementary motor area, L = Left, R = Right. P<.005 uncorrected. Minimal cluster size k = 50. * Significant peak at p<.001 uncorrected.

MS patients > HC comparisons

During viewing of negative scenes, MS patients showed higher recruitment of more ventral prefrontal areas, including orbital IFG and subgenual ACC, together with the left caudate nucleus (p <0.005; fig. 2, table 4A). During reappraisal of negative scenes, these same ventral prefrontal areas (including orbital IFG and subgenual ACC) were again more recruited in patients, together with the intra-parietal sulcus (IPS) and occipito-temporal visual areas (p <0.005; fig. 2, table 4C; see also fig. 3A). When viewing positive scenes, MS patients showed higher activity in subgenual ACC, caudate nucleus, and visual areas, whereas increase in activity was restricted to visual areas during reappraisal of these scenes (p <0.005; fig. 1, table 4, B and D).
Figure 2
MS patients > healthy controls (HC), whole-brain contrast during the viewing and reappraisal of negative and positive emotional scenes (p <0.005 uncorrected, cluster size >50). L = left hemisphere, R = right hemisphere.
Table 4
MS patients > Controls comparison of whole-brain activity during emotion viewing and reappraisal of negative scenes.
 BA  T-value  MNI coordinates  Cluster size
x   y   z
A. VIEWING NEGATIVE SCENES      
FRONTAL / SUB-CORTICAL / INSULAR      
L orbital IFG*47    3.39-50  28  -8106
R orbital IFG*47    3.2350  34  -1450
L subgenual ACC32    2.99-8  42  -652
L caudate nucleus*     3.40-18  22   2110
R anterior insula38    3.0536  12  -1656
R temporal pole     2.7036  12  -26 
VISUAL      
L superior occipital gyrus*19    3.41-24-82  3457
R inferior temporal gyrus*37    3.4140-66  -658
R middle occipital gyrus*19    4.0936-86  20125
B. VIEWING POSITIVE SCENES      
FRONTAL / SUB-CORTICAL      
L caudate nucleus     3.00-18  22   4111
L subgenual ACC32    2.82-12  38  -8 
C. REAPRAISAL OF NEGATIVE SCENES      
FRONTAL      
L orbital IFG*47    3.51-36  30  -888
R subgenual ACC*32    3.358  36  -1469
PARIETAL      
L intra-parietal sulcus*     3.55-24-46  4070
VISUAL      
L superior occipital gyrus*19    3.64-22-74  26144
R superior occipital gyrus*19    3.8422-86  1896
R middle occipital gyrus18/19    3.1236-86  20 
D. REAPRAISAL OF POSITIVE SCENES      
VISUAL      
L superior occipital gyrus*19    3.21-22-82  3250
R superior occipital gyrus*19    3.2822-86  1879
R middle occipital gyrus     3.0230-70  26 
BA = Brodmann areas, IFG = inferior frontal gyrus, ACC = anterior cingulate cortex, L = Left, R = Right. P<.005 uncorrected. Minimal cluster size k = 50. * Significant peak at p<.001 uncorrected.
Figure 3
Functional connectivity analysis of orbital IFG. (A) The left orbital IFG is more recruited in MS patients compared with healthy controls (HC) during the reappraisal of negative emotions. (B) A functional connectivity analysis with the left orbital IFG as seed indicates higher connectivity with bilateral amygdala in MS compared with HC (p <0.005). (C) In MS patients, the magnitude of activation within the left amygdala correlates positively with alexithymia scores at the TAS-2 (difficulty identifying feeling; Pearson correlation). L = left hemisphere.

Functional connectivity analyses.

The orbital IFG was selectively engaged in MS patients (fig. 3A) and has been implicated in the regulation of amygdala activity in the normal population [10, 17]. Therefore, we examined its modulation and functional coupling with other areas involved in emotion reappraisal. Connectivity analysis showed increased functional coupling in patients compared with controls between the left orbital IFG and the left amygdala on the one hand (MNI coordinates: −24, −6, −10; p = 0.015 FWE-corrected, small volume correction; fig. 3B, table 5), and also with the right amygdala (MNI coordinates: 26, −4, −20; p = 0.021 FWE-corrected, small volume correction; table 5). The reverse comparison did not reveal any significant group difference. To explore whether amygdala activity was abnormally regulated in alexithymic patients, we performed a complementary correlation analysis revealing that activity within the left amygdala was positively associated with the “difficulty identifying one’s feeling” alexithymic symptom in MS patients (TAS-2 subscale; rho = 0.599; p = 0.007; fig. 3C). This was also the case for the right amygdala (TAS-2 subscale; rho = 0.468; p = 0.044).
Table 5
Functional connectivity analysis with the left orbital IFG as seed region (MNI coordinates: -36, 30, -8) during the reappraisal of negative scenes within the emotion regulation network defined by Neurosynth.
   BAT-Value    MNI coordinates   Cluster size
   X  Y  Z
MS patients > HC      
L amygdala 3.02-24-6-1017
      2.98  -30  -2-18 
R amygdala     2.87   26  -4-2013
L superior medial frontal gyrus   32    3.45  -10  40  1813
HC > MS patients-      -    -   -   -     -
     p<.005 uncorrected, minimum cluster size = 10 voxels. MS = multiple sclerosis, HC = healthy controls. L = Left, R = Right.

Discussion

Functional MRI was applied to investigate the brain mechanisms of emotion perception and regulation in minimally disabled MS patients, together with their relationship with alexithymia.

Alexithymia: link with questionnaires, disease progression and white matter damage

Alexithymic disorders, which reflect difficulties in recognising and reporting one’s own emotions, were highly prevalent in the present sample of MS patients, in line with previous epidemiological studies [12]. Alexithymia total scores were associated with higher disease duration, disability and depression, as previously reported [3739]. Moreover, alexithymia was correlated with the extent of white matter damage as assessed by whole-brain white matter volumetry, corroborating another recent study [24].

Emotional ratings

Across groups, emotional ratings were strongly related with whole-brain volumetric measures of white matter. In particular, higher white matter volumes were associated with more efficient emotional regulation during viewing of both negative and positive emotions. Thus, a rich network of white matter tracts supporting communication between distant brain areas is likely to support effective regulation strategies (e.g [40].), but also vulnerable to white matter lesions due to MS. In accordance with two other studies examining the association between alexithymia and emotion recognition, we did not observe any correlation between emotion ratings and alexithymia scores in patients [14, 37]. Moreover, average emotional ratings did not differ between patients and controls. One reason could be that the patients were only slightly impacted by the disease, owing to relatively short disease durations. Indeed, in our sample, the median EDSS score was 1.5. In previous studies with similar scores, no impairment was observed in emotion perception [7, 41]. Conversely, in a study involving patients with higher EDSS scores, impaired emotion perception was associated with longer disease durations [6].
Although these correlations provide indirect support to the notion that alexithymia may ultimately reflect impaired structural connectivity within distributed brain networks underlying the top-down regulation of emotional processes, we did not perform more detailed analysis using diffusion tensor imaging to identify the role of specific white-matter tracts. This limitation should be addressed in future follow up studies in MS patients and other neurological populations.

Compensatory activity in the orbital IFG during emotion perception and regulation

We hypothesised a dysfunctional recruitment of the lateral PFC in patients, due to its involvement in the regulation of amygdala activity during reappraisal. Remarkably, the ventrolateral PFC (BA47) was more activated in patients than controls during emotion reappraisal. This region is sensitive to emotional valence [10, 17] and has been repeatedly involved in emotion regulation [10, 19]. More particularly, it is part of an “external emotion control” pathway implicated in the modulation of amygdala activity following external emotional stimuli [42]. A second circuit also converges towards the amygdala, the “internal emotion control” pathway; in contrast to the former, this circuit modulates internally triggered affective states and implicates the orbito-frontal cortex instead (BA11). Abnormal functional connectivity within the external emotion control pathway has been previously documented in mood disorders [40, 42], similar to the present findings.
The ventrolateral PFC has also been implicated in past studies of emotion perception in MS, and was found to be more recruited in unimpaired than impaired MS patients or healthy controls [5, 6], in accordance with the present data. Here, we further observed increased functional connectivity between ventrolateral PFC and the amygdala in MS patients during the reappraisal of negative scenes. Previously, the ventrolateral PFC has been related with initiation [15], selection and inhibition mechanisms during reappraisal [16]. Because amygdala activity did not globally differ from controls, at least as revealed by the whole-brain analysis, our data might reflect an increased top-down input from the ventrolateral PFC towards the amygdala, aimed at normalising the activity of the latter region [10, 17]. In the past, this connectivity was found to be decreased in MS in the presence of increased activity in ventrolateral PFC during emotion identification [6]. More recently, damage to the uncinate fasciculus linking ventrolateral PFC with the amygdala has been implicated in social cognition disorders in MS [3]. From a functional point of view, which of the two mechanisms (increase or decrease of functional connectivity) is observed in a given study may depend on the effects of task demands and characteristics.

Relationship between amygdala activity and alexithymia in MS patients

Among our main findings, we report a significant positive association in patients between amygdala activity and alexithymia severity (subscale scores for difficulty identifying feelings) during the reappraisal of negative scenes. This might point to decreased down-regulation of limbic activity with increasing alexithymia. Similarly, in a psychiatric sample of patients featuring emotional lability, activity in ventrolateral PFC as well as in a number of limbic areas increased during regulation of negative emotions [38]. The amygdala is well known to react reflexively to negative stimuli [39]. During successful reappraisal, the emotional impact of negative events is decreased thanks to a down-modulation of amygdala activity [10, 1719]. A less efficient regulation strategy, emotional suppression, is on the contrary known to increase amygdala activity [17]. In our sample of patients, increased amygdala activity might thus reflect a deficit in applying a reappraisal strategy in the face of negative emotions, which would be commensurate with alexithymia level.

Abnormal subgenual ACC activity in MS patients

The subgenual ACC (or rostral ACC) was also globally more activated in patients, both during emotion perception and reappraisal, and irrespective of emotional valence. This region is a key part of the affective division of ACC, involved in the production of parasympathetic responses [43], as opposed to a more dorsal cognitive pathway [41, 44]. This affective ACC works together with other components of the limbic system (insular cortex and amygdala, notably) for the identification of emotional significance, whereas a more dorsal pathway including the dorsal aspect of ACC appears more related to the regulation of an emotional state evaluation [41, 44]. Here, a mobilisation of this affective component suggests additional compensation mechanisms that might aim at detecting emotional states in patients.
Overall, our results converge with recent meta-analyses suggesting that neural alterations in several limbic regions in alexithymic disorders, including amygdala, striatum, orbitofrontal, ventromedial and prefrontal areas [47]. However, it worth nothing that we did not observe significant group differences in the insula as reported in these previous studies, although this region was normally responsive to emotion in both controls and patients. Though speculative, this might argue against a key role of the insula in mediating the access of affective signals to conscious experience and deficits thereof in alexithymia, as speculated by some theoretical models of emotional awareness [48], but instead points to abnormal modulation of affective processing due to altered top-down control circuits. This conclusion, however, requires replication in other populations.

Limitations

Our data should be interpreted with caution insofar as a relatively small sample of MS patients was included in the present study. However, we had strict selection criteria in order to recruit patients with only minimal neurological deficits, no major cognitive deficit and short disease duration. Moreover, our population appeared highly comparable to those reported in other MS studies. In the future, a larger cohort including different disease stages should be used in order to replicate as well as better understand the association between changes in functional connectivity, disease stage and emotion regulation impact on affective well-being, as well as their development. Ultimately, better understanding of how demyelinating lesions impact patients’ ability to monitor their emotional state constitutes a crucial line of research to improve mental health care in MS.
Mitsouko van Assche, PhD, Neurology Unit, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva, University Hospitals, Rue Gabrielle-Perret-Gentil 4, CH-1211 Geneva 4, mitsouko.vanassche[at]gmail.com
1 Social cognition in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2016;87(16):1727–36. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003236 PubMed
2 Disconnection mechanism and regional cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: a structural MRI study. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e82422. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082422 PubMed
3 Disconnection as a mechanism for social cognition impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. 2017;89(1):38–45. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000004060 PubMed
4 Dulau C. Social Cognition and Multiple Sclerosis. In: Brochet B, editor. Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Inflammatory Demyelinating Diseases. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2015. p. 213-26.
5 Emotional disturbances in multiple sclerosis: A neuropsychological and fMRI study. Cortex. 2019;117:205–16. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.017 PubMed
6 Prefrontal function associated with impaired emotion recognition in patients with multiple sclerosis. Behav Brain Res. 2009;205(1):280–5. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.009 PubMed
7 Neurobiological mechanisms underlying emotional processing in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Brain. 2009;132(12):3380–91. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp095 PubMed
8 . Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: from “what is it?” to “what’s to be done?”. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(12):3416–29. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038 PubMed
9 Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed by masked angry faces: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2004;21(1):352–63. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.021 PubMed
10 . Common and distinct brain networks underlying explicit emotional evaluation: a meta-analytic study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7(5):521–34. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp001 PubMed
11 . Alexithymia in Neurological Disease: A Review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015;27(3):179–87. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14070169 PubMed
12 . Alexithymia in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review of literature. Neuropsychologia. 2017;104:31–47. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.034 PubMed
13 . Emotional dysfunctions in neurodegenerative diseases. J Comp Neurol. 2016;524(8):1727–43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.23816 PubMed
14 Alexithymia and impaired facial affect recognition in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol. 2011;258(9):1683–8. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00415-011-6002-4 PubMed
15 . Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation--an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Neuroimage. 2014;87:345–55. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001 PubMed
16 Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(11):2981–90. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht154 PubMed
17 . The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(11):693–700. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn4044 PubMed
18 . The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):577–86. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031 PubMed
19 . The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(5):242–9. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010 PubMed
20 . Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037–50. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006 PubMed
21 The relationship of alexithymia to emotional dysregulation within an alcohol dependent treatment sample. Addict Behav. 2012;37(4):469–76. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.12.011 PubMed
22 . Emotion dysregulation in alexithymia: Startle reactivity to fearful affective imagery and its relation to heart rate variability. Psychophysiology. 2017;54(9):1323–34. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12887 PubMed
23 Neural correlates of alexithymia: a meta-analysis of emotion processing studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1774–85. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.008 PubMed
24 The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: a consensus model. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(4):313–25. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01022.x PubMed
25 . Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(5):504–14. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00168-9 PubMed
26 . Neural substrates of decision making as measured with the Iowa Gambling Task in men with alexithymia. Psychosom Med. 2011;73(7):588–97. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e318223c7f8 PubMed
27 . Perturbation emotionnelles en neurologie et en psychiatrie: Humeur et émotions dans la sclérose en plaques. Neuropsy News.2003;2(3):91–6.
28 . Effects of emotion regulation strategy on brain responses to the valence and social content of visual scenes. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(5):1067–82. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.020 PubMed
29 . The neural substrates of social emotion perception and regulation are modulated by adult attachment style. Soc Neurosci. 2012;7(5):473–93. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.647410 PubMed
30 Polman CH, Reingold SC, Edan G, Filippi M, Hartung HP, Kappos L, et al. (2005). Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2005 revisions to the “McDonald Criteria”. Ann Neurol. 2005,58(6):840-6.
31 . Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology. 1983;33(11):1444–52. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444 PubMed
32 . The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale--I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38(1):23–32. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1 PubMed
33 Rao SM. A manual for the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests in multiple sclerosis. Milwaukee: Medical College of Wisconsin; 1990.
34 The brief repeatable battery of neuropsychological tests: normative values allow application in multiple sclerosis clinical practice. Mult Scler. 2001;7(4):263–7. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/135245850100700409 PubMed
35 . A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):1277–86. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068 PubMed
36 . Longitudinal study of alexithymia and multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav. 2014;4(1):75–82. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.194 PubMed
37 Facial and bodily emotion recognition in multiple sclerosis: the role of alexithymia and other characteristics of the disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(10):1004–14. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000939 PubMed
38 Alexithymia in multiple sclerosis: relationship with fatigue and depression. Acta Neurol Scand. 2008;118(1):18–23. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2007.00969.x PubMed
39 . Predictors of depression in multiple sclerosis patients. Acta Neurol Scand. 2010;121(3):161–70. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01232.x PubMed
40 White matter abnormalities in first-episode, treatment-naive young adults with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(5):823–6. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.5.823 PubMed
41 Cognitively preserved MS patients demonstrate functional differences in processing neutral and emotional faces. Brain Imaging Behav. 2011;5(4):241–51. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11682-011-9128-1 PubMed
42 Alexithymia and reduced white matter integrity in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study on impaired emotional self-awareness. Schizophr Res. 2012;141(2-3):137–43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.026 PubMed
43 . Damage to association fiber tracts impairs recognition of the facial expression of emotion. J Neurosci. 2009;29(48):15089–99. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0796-09.2009 PubMed
44 Resting-state functional connectivity of emotion regulation networks in euthymic and non-euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Eur Psychiatry. 2016;34:56–63. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.12.005 PubMed
47 . Structure of the alexithymic brain: A parametric coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:50–5. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.004 PubMed
48 . Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:594–611. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.007 PubMed